How Do Great Apes Keep Their Babies Clean From Urine and Feces

1. Introduction

Olfaction is among the oldest senses [1], and influences social behaviour such as territorial defence, kin recognition and mate choice in many species [2,3]. In contrast to other mammals, research on olfaction in the primate order was neglected for decades, mostly because primates were considered to be 'microsmatic' (having a poor sense of smell), relying more on visual and acoustic rather than olfactory cues [4,5]. The notion that primates are microsmatic was primarily based on anatomical evidence such as a reduction in the proportional size of the olfactory epithelium and olfactory bulb volume as compared with most other mammalian species [6,7] or a decrease in functional olfactory receptor genes [8,9]. However, neuroanatomical features are not necessarily correlated with olfactory performance [5].

Good olfactory capabilities are now recognized in some primate taxa, but not in others. Strepsirrhines (lemurs, lorises and galagos) and platyrrhines (New World monkeys) rely heavily on olfactory communication, showing classical scent marking behaviour and/or possessing specialized scent glands [10,11]. For example, callitrichids use olfactory cues to detect ovulation [12] and familiar individuals [13], and lemurs signal information including identity and relatedness in their glandular secretions, which conspecifics can perceive in experiments [14–16]. In contrast to strepsirrhines and New World monkeys, catarrhines (Old World monkeys and apes, including humans) do not seem to have a functional vomeronasal organ (VNO) and accessory olfactory bulb (structures related to the perception of pheromones), at least postnatally [17]. However, the main olfactory system may also perceive social signals [18], and recent evidence suggests that social odorants serve a signalling function in Old World monkeys (e.g. [19–26]). Evidence for human chemo-signalling is also growing, indicating that olfaction may play a greater role in humans than previously assumed [27,28]. For example, humans have excellent odour discrimination abilities [29] and an unexpectedly high olfactory sensitivity that exceeds that of mice, rats and dogs for some substances [30]. Furthermore, humans may use chemosignals in mate choice [31,32] and can recognize kin via body odour alone [33–35].

Given the importance of olfaction in other primates including humans, the scarcity of research on the sense of smell in non-human great apes (hereafter apes) is surprising and leaves an important gap in our understanding of primate chemical communication. Although observations suggest that they use their sense of smell in various contexts [36], can discriminate between natural odours [37] and detect contaminants [38,39], no experimental investigations yet exist examining which social information apes perceive via odours. Human rater studies suggest, however, that gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) produce individually identifiable body odours [40] and that wild gorilla silverbacks use body odour as a flexible, context-dependent signalling mechanism to group members and outgroup conspecifics [41].

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are one of our two closest living relatives and an important model species for understanding human evolution [42]. Wild chimpanzees sniff their environment: males sniff more frequently in sexual and social contexts, and females more frequently in a feeding context [43]. Chimpanzees also often sniff the ground, vegetation and signs of chimpanzees such as nests, faeces and urine while patrolling the borders of their territory [44,45]. However, the function of sniffing behaviour during boundary patrols is unclear. Since chimpanzees live in multi-male, multi-female communities with high fission–fusion dynamics, where community members may not meet for several days [46,47], and since chimpanzees are highly territorial and often kill individuals from other communities [44,48], obtaining information about the whereabouts of group members and outgroup individuals and the ability to discriminate between the latter are crucial for maximizing fitness, and it seems likely that they use olfaction to perceive this information.

In addition to recognizing group members, chimpanzees may maximize inclusive fitness through kin recognition, which is important to avoid inbreeding [49] and prevent infanticide [50]. Chimpanzees bias their behaviour towards and form strong social bonds with relatives [51–54] (but see [55]), and breed with genetically dissimilar mates [56], suggesting they can recognize their kin. Although they have excellent facial recognition abilities [57,58], olfactory cues may also be important for kin recognition in chimpanzees, given their fission–fusion behaviour and often dense habitat.

We report behavioural bioassays investigating the signalling function of social odours in apes. Our first aim was to test whether chimpanzees can distinguish between group members (ingroup individuals) and non-group members (outgroup individuals) using olfactory cues. In a simultaneous discrimination task, we presented two groups of zoo-living chimpanzees with urine from ingroup and outgroup individuals and an unscented control. If chimpanzees can discriminate between in- and outgroup urine, they should show a differential behavioural response towards the odour stimuli. We predicted that chimpanzees will investigate urine longer than the unscented control, and outgroup urine longer than ingroup urine. Our second aim was to investigate whether chimpanzees can recognize their kin via olfactory cues. Since chimpanzees bias their behaviour towards relatives, we predicted that they will respond more strongly towards kin than non-kin odour.

2. Material and methods

(a) Study site and population

We studied two groups of captive chimpanzees (groups A and B) at the Wolfgang Köhler Primate Research Center at Leipzig Zoo from December 2015 to February 2016. At the time of the study, group A contained 18 (11 females and seven males) and group B seven (six females and one male) individuals. We excluded one male infant in group A because its behavioural responses were influenced by its mother. The subjects' age range was 6–50 years (mean ± s.d. = 25 ± 13 years; electronic supplementary material 2, table S1). Each group has access to spacious inside and outside enclosures with regular feeding, daily enrichment and water available ad libitum.

(b) Pilot study on odour source preference

In a pilot study, we tested which odour source chimpanzees respond most strongly to by presenting three different odour sources (body odour, faeces and urine) from the same group member to both groups. Chimpanzees reacted most strongly towards urine, so we used urine as an odour source for subsequent bioassays. The increased inspection time for urine compared to faeces might reflect a trade-off between gathering information about the scent donor and infection risk from faeces (see electronic supplementary material 1 for detailed methods, results and discussion).

(c) Urine collection

We collected urine samples from adult chimpanzees in both groups at Leipzig Zoo using disposable pipettes directly after animals urinated on the floor in the sleeping or observation rooms and stored them in 15 ml plastic tubes at −20°C until use. We used new pipettes for each individual and only collected urine that we could assign to an individual. For females, we only collected urine when they showed no sexual swelling and were not menstruating, to avoid hormonal influences of the menstrual cycle on the odour.

(d) Behavioural bioassay

To test whether chimpanzees can discriminate between ingroup and outgroup individuals via olfactory cues, we presented urine from one ingroup individual and one outgroup individual and an unscented control (to test whether animals perceive urine over background) simultaneously 1 m apart in three Plexiglas boxes (12 × 12 × 20 cm3) installed on a metal grid on the ground in the inside enclosure (electronic supplementary material 2, figure S1). The odourless boxes were locked with padlocks, had multiple holes (3 mm in diameter) on each side and were lined with odourless wire mesh (mesh size: 0.5 mm) to prevent chimpanzees from using sticks to reach the contents. Thirty minutes prior to each session, we thawed samples and the experimenter (SH) rated their odour intensity on a scale of 0–10 to control for a potential influence of odour intensity on response behaviours. We placed 15 ml of each urine sample on a small piece of 100% cotton in the test boxes and a piece of cotton without urine in the control box. We always handled boxes with odourless disposable vinyl gloves to avoid transferring human body odour to the test apparatus.

We installed boxes in the morning before the chimpanzees entered the inside enclosure and removed them as soon as the animals left the enclosure (session duration 7 h 18 m–8 h 33 m, mean ± s.d. = 8 h 8 m ± 20 m). All group members present in the inside enclosure could visit the boxes at any time, a more natural situation than testing subjects individually. Although this means that data for individual animals may depend on that of other animals, our observations did not suggest that this was the case. After each session, the experimenter assigned a new intensity score to each sample to assess changes in odour intensity over time. We disinfected boxes for an hour with a 4% solution of aldehyde-free antiseptic cleaner (OROsept K, Kleen Purgatis GmbH) then rinsed them thoroughly with water after each session.

We counterbalanced the location of the three stimuli (ingroup, outgroup and control) to control for potential side preferences. We used urine from 15 different individuals (10 female and 5 male) and conducted six sessions per group: three sessions with only female odours and three sessions with only male odours (we assigned sex order randomly). We conducted sessions at intervals of at least 6 days (range = 6–17 days, mean ± s.d. = 8.9 ± 3.8 days) to reduce habituation effects.

We recorded the chimpanzees' behavioural reactions towards the odour stimuli using a digital video camera (Panasonic HC-V757 HD), positioned on an observer platform outside the enclosure.

(e) Video analysis and behavioural definitions

S.H. analysed videos frame-by-frame using Mangold Interact v. 16.1.0 (see electronic supplementary material 3 for example video). We recorded the durations and time stamps of response behaviours. Our main target response behaviour was sniffing (defined as placing the nose within 3 cm of the box), but we also included the olfaction-related responses nose within 20 cm and licking, and the non-olfaction-related responses presence within 50 cm, touching and manipulating (electronic supplementary material 2, table S2). For analysis, we combined touching and manipulating into one response variable (manipulating) to measure tactile investigation of odours. To determine the mode of first investigation, we used the first behaviour shown when an animal first arrived at the boxes. To gain insight into whether chimpanzees perceive and discriminate odours from more than 20 cm, we recorded which odour stimulus the subjects sniffed first. The video camera automatically split the recordings into 22 min segments. To estimate inter-observer reliability, a second rater coded 23 of 144 (16%) video segments used for statistical analysis, representing all sessions in both groups. Both raters were blind to the location of the odour stimuli. We estimated inter-observer reliability using Spearman's rank correlations for the total durations of each behaviour per individual, box location and session [59]. Where n was smaller than 10, we used the exact version of Spearman's rank correlation (r s: range = 0.346–1, mean ± s.d. = 0.733 ± 0.191; see electronic supplementary material, S2 and table S3 for detailed results).

(f) Statistical analysis

We conducted all analyses using linear mixed models (LMMs) [60] with Gaussian error structure and identity link. We set significance at p < 0.05 and trends at 0.05 ≤ p < 0.1. We fitted all models in R (v. 3.4.0) [61] using function lmer of R package lme4 (v. 1.1–13) [62].

To test whether chimpanzees react differently to urine odours compared with the control and whether they show a differential behavioural response to ingroup and outgroup odours, we fitted two separate LMMs for each response variable. We used the total duration of behaviours (per subject, odour stimulus and session) as response variables and odour stimulus as test predictor. We included sex, group and age of the subject, session number and box location (for all models) and sex and group of the odour donor and intensity score (for ingroup versus outgroup models) as control predictors. To account for a potential differential response of males and females towards male and female odours in the ingroup versus outgroup models, we also included the three-way interaction between odour stimulus, subject sex and odour donor sex. We included subject ID and session ID (for all models) and odour ID (for ingroup versus outgroup models) as random effects and used a maximal random slopes structure.

To test whether the degree of relatedness influences behavioural responses, we fitted an LMM for each response variable for ingroup odours only. We calculated relatedness coefficients (range = 0–0.5) from pedigree data kindly provided by Leipzig Zoo. We included the relatedness coefficient as test predictor and all control predictors that were significant for the ingroup versus outgroup models. We included subject ID, odour ID and session ID as random effects and used a maximal random slopes structure (see electronic supplementary material 2 for details of statistical analyses).

3. Results

(a) Mode of first investigation

In 83% (73/88) of cases, the first behaviour chimpanzees showed after first approaching a box (apart from looking) was related to olfaction (sniffing, nose within 20 cm, licking); in 15% (13/88) of cases, chimpanzees used tactile investigation first, and in 2% (2/88) of cases they only inspected the boxes visually while present within 50 cm (figure 1). Two adult males (α-male and ex-α-male of group A) never investigated any of the boxes, possibly because they were the oldest males in the group (22 and 40 years) and olfactory function decreases with age (e.g. [63,64]). None of the encounters with the boxes produced any obvious alarm or aggressive responses from the subjects.

Figure 1.

Figure 1. First investigatory behaviours after chimpanzees first approached a box in a test session. Olfactory investigation includes sniffing, nose within 20 cm and licking; tactile investigation includes touching and manipulating; visual investigation includes just presence within 50 cm (paying attention to box without olfactory or tactile investigation).

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download PowerPoint

(b) First sniffs and number of individuals sniffing

Chimpanzees sniffed more often at outgroup odours first upon their first approach, and this was most apparent in the first two sessions (table 1).

Table 1. Number of individuals that sniffed first at a given odour stimulus and the number of individuals that sniffed all three odours per session.

Collapse

session 1 2 3 4 5 6 total observations
number of individuals that sniffed first at a given odour
outgroup 11 10 2 3 4 2 32
ingroup 4 4 7 8 2 4 29
control 5 4 2 2 6 5 24
total 20 18 11 13 12 11 85
number of individuals that sniffed all three odours
9 5 3 1 1 1 20

The number of individuals that sniffed a box declined from 20 in the first session to 11 in the last session (table 1). The number of subjects that sniffed all three boxes in a session decreased from nine individuals in the first session to one individual in sessions 4–6 (table 1).

(c) Control versus odour

Our control versus odour models revealed a significant influence of the set of predictor variables on the total time individuals spent sniffing and present within 50 cm of a stimulus, but not of nose within 20 cm, licking or manipulating (electronic supplementary material, S2 and table S9). More specifically, subjects sniffed odour stimuli and stayed within 50 cm of them for significantly longer than they did for control stimuli (figure 2 and table 2; for detailed results of all predictor variables see electronic supplementary material 2, table S10). Session number had a highly significant negative effect on the duration of sniffing and tended to have a negative effect on presence within 50 cm (figure 3; electronic supplementary material 2, table S10), indicating habituation over the six sessions. Younger individuals spent significantly more time within 50 cm of the boxes than older individuals (electronic supplementary material 2, table S10).

Figure 2.

Figure 2. Total durations of response behaviours towards urine odour samples and an unscented control. Plots show the median (thick horizontal lines) and quartiles (boxes) for sniffing and presence within 50 cm. Data are presented on a log scale.

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download PowerPoint
Figure 3.

Figure 3. Effect of session number on the total duration of sniffing and presence within 50 cm. Dashed lines depict the model (fitted based on all fixed effects manually dummy coded and then centred to a mean of zero), thin dotted lines the 95% confidence intervals of the model. Data are presented on a log scale.

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download PowerPoint

Table 2. Results of main effects for linear mixed models with Gaussian error structure testing the influence of stimulus type on chimpanzees' response to odour stimuli. For results of control predictors see electronic supplementary material 2, tables S10–S12. Significance and trends are marked in italics. Ref, reference level. Colons represent the interaction between fixed effects.

Collapse

model response variable predictor variable estimate s.e. χ 2 p-value
control versus odour sniffing odour (ref = control) 0.482 0.213 4.660 0 . 031
nose within 20 cm odour (ref = control) 0.362 0.243 1.913 0.167
licking odour (ref = control) 0.382 0.293 1.572 0.210
presence within 50 cm odour (ref = control) 0.507 0.234 3.961 0 . 047
Manipulating odour (ref = control) 0.393 0.274 1.985 0.159
ingroup versus outgroup sniffing odour (ref = ingroup) 0.564 0.251 4.675 0 . 031
nose within 20 cm odour : subject sex : odour sex −0.820 1.246 0.423 0.515
licking odour : subject sex −0.222 0.7696 0.101 0.751
odour : odour sex −0.560 0.851 0.419 0.517
presence within 50 cm odour : subject sex : odour sex −2.718 1.413 2.975 0 . 085
manipulating odour : subject sex 1.605 0.735 3.981 0 . 046
odour : odour sex 0.032 0.886 0.001 0.972
relatedness sniffing r a 0.544 0.226 5.152 0 . 023
nose within 20 cm r a 1.839 1.506 1.461 0.227
licking r a 0.316 0.351 0.762 0.383
presence within 50 cm r a 0.791 0.246 8.719 0 . 003
manipulating r a : subject sex 0.429 0.414 1.040 0.308

(d) Ingroup versus outgroup

The results of the ingroup versus outgroup models revealed that the set of predictor variables tended to influence sniffing but not nose within 20 cm, licking, presence within 50 cm or manipulating (electronic supplementary material 2, table S9). The three-way interaction between odour stimulus, sex of subject ID and sex of odour ID and the three two-way interactions were not significant for sniffing (tested by fitting a reduced model without the three-way interaction), so we fitted a reduced model without interactions to obtain interpretable p-values for the main effects. The reduced model revealed a significant effect of odour stimulus, with subjects sniffing outgroup odours longer than ingroup odours (figure 4 and table 2; electronic supplementary material 2, table S11).

Figure 4.

Figure 4. Total duration of sniffing of ingroup and outgroup urine. Plots show the median (thick horizontal lines) and quartiles (boxes). Data are presented on a log scale.

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download PowerPoint

(e) Relatedness

When we tested the effect of relatedness on behavioural responses towards familiar individuals, we found a significant influence of the set of predictor variables on the total durations of sniffing and presence within 50 cm but not of nose within 20 cm, licking and manipulating (electronic supplementary material 2, table S9). The total time spent sniffing and present within 50 cm of familiar odours increased with relatedness to the odour donor (figure 5 and table 2; electronic supplementary material 2, table S12).

Figure 5.

Figure 5. Effect of relatedness, expressed as the relatedness coefficient, on the total duration of sniffing and presence within 50 cm. Dashed lines depict the model (fitted based on subject group manually dummy coded and then centred to a mean of zero), thin dotted lines the 95% confidence intervals of the model. Data are presented on a log scale.

  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download PowerPoint

4. Discussion

We present bioassays testing the effect of conspecific odours on the behaviour of non-human apes. Overall, our results support our predictions, and suggest that chimpanzees are sensitive to social odorants and that they detect group membership and relatedness via olfactory cues.

(a) Control versus odour and mode of first investigation

Chimpanzees sniffed urine stimuli for significantly longer and stayed within 50 cm of them for longer than they did for the control. This finding suggests that chimpanzees are sensitive to social odorants [65] and reflects responses to urinary stimuli in macaques [66]. However, chimpanzees also showed some interest in and sniffed the control box, suggesting they use their sense of smell as a general investigation mechanism for novel objects. Supporting this hypothesis, 83% of the first behaviours shown when approaching the boxes for the first time in a session were related to olfaction. Thus, our results highlight the long-neglected importance of olfaction in this species.

(b) Ingroup versus outgroup

Chimpanzees sniffed outgroup urine significantly longer than ingroup urine. Since differences in responsiveness between odorants typically reflect discriminability [5,65], our results suggest that chimpanzees can discriminate between group members and outgroup conspecifics using olfactory cues. Our result is in line with findings for several other mammalian species (e.g. [67–70]), including primates. Strepsirrhines [71,72], New World monkeys [13,73] and Old World monkeys [19] discriminate between ingroup and outgroup individuals, responding for longer or more strongly to the scent of familiar than unfamiliar conspecifics. Our findings show that this discrimination mechanism also exists in apes, strengthening existing evidence that the perception of social olfactory signals does not depend on a functional VNO.

Sustained inspection of an unfamiliar outgroup odour may reflect a higher interest in a novel odour or increased efforts to obtain and process new information about the individual scent donor [68]. According to the scent-matching hypothesis, individuals of territorial species learn the scent of marks they encounter in the environment and then compare it with the scent of animals they meet, to facilitate appropriate behaviour [74,75]. Besides obtaining information about the state or identity of an intruder, chimpanzees might also be able to assess the distance to competing groups through the freshness or intensity of the odour or the frequency of outgroup scents encountered [74]. Although chimpanzees are not known to actively scent mark their territory, they sniff and inspect olfactory cues such as urine, faeces or traces of body odour in chimpanzee nests on boundary patrols [44,45]. Our findings suggest that they use these cues to identify the trail of group members and gather information about intruders, and thus to maximize fitness, for example by reducing aggression.

The ability to discriminate between group members and outgroup individuals via odour may be based on familiarity with or individual recognition of the scent donor. Familiarity alone cannot explain our results, because chimpanzees also detected their degree of relatedness to the urine donor, suggesting that they respond differently based on information encoded in the odour, in addition to familiarity. Whether this information is based on the particular odour donor and thus reflects a mental representation of individual scents in chimpanzees cannot be reliably deciphered based on our findings but opens up an interesting area for future research.

Chemical studies using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry also suggest that chemical cues contain information about group membership in many species (e.g. [76–78]). In primates, chemical profiles reflect group membership in scent gland secretions in mandrills, Mandrillus sphinx [20] and body odour in rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta [21]. Given our results, it is likely that chimpanzee urinary olfactory profiles also encode information about group membership and kinship [79]. Chemical analyses should investigate the information content (including, e.g. sex, age, dominance status, reproductive state) and chemical composition of chimpanzee urine.

(c) Habituation

Our results suggest that chimpanzees habituated to the odour stimuli over the six sessions. Durations of sniffing decreased significantly and the number of animals that sniffed one or more boxes declined considerably, suggesting a loss of interest in the boxes. Future studies of olfactory discrimination abilities in chimpanzees should therefore limit the number of sessions to a maximum of three sessions if three scent stimuli are involved (so that stimulus placement can still be counterbalanced) and rather increase the number of groups tested or apply a habituation–dishabituation paradigm (e.g. [37]).

Many more individuals sniffed outgroup odours first than ingroup or control stimuli in the first two sessions. In the majority of these cases, chimpanzees approached the box containing the outgroup odour straight after entering the inside enclosure, whereas in subsequent sessions they often ignored the boxes on entering the enclosure. They may have quickly realized that there is no real danger of the presence of intruders and thus lost interest in the stimuli. Although the number of first sniffs of outgroup odours was not considerably higher than for ingroup or control stimuli over the six sessions, given our observations in the first two sessions and the strong habituation effect suggesting the first few sessions are most important, we cannot rule out the possibility that chimpanzees perceive important olfactory cues from a greater distance. Further studies of olfactory sensitivity thresholds in chimpanzees are needed to shed more light on their olfactory capabilities.

(d) Relatedness

The more closely related chimpanzees were to a familiar group member, the longer they sniffed and stayed within 50 cm of that individual's urine stimulus. This suggests that chimpanzees can perceive the degree of relatedness through urinary olfactory cues and that such cues may play a crucial role in kin recognition in this species. Our results are in line with a recent study showing that chimpanzees breed with genetically dissimilar mates and take the degree of relatedness into account in their mate choices [56]. Enhanced inspection of familiar kin versus familiar non-kin may reflect generally greater interest in related individuals who are often preferred affiliation and coalition partners [46,53,55,80].

Maternal kin recognition in chimpanzees is well accepted, and is likely to be based on prior association with the mother [46,52,53,55,80,81]. However, whether chimpanzees can recognize paternal kin is contentious [53,55], although recent evidence that fathers associate more with their offspring than with unrelated infants provides evidence that they can [44,47]. Recognition of paternal relatives in polygynandrous species probably depends on phenotype matching based on visual, acoustic or olfactory cues (reviewed in [50,82]). Chimpanzees' ability to assess kinship visually through facial recognition is well established [57,58,83]. Our findings suggest that phenotype matching via olfactory cues is a further mechanism for kin recognition in chimpanzees. However, since our dataset was too small to differentiate between maternal and paternal kin dyads, future studies should investigate whether chimpanzees can use odour to distinguish maternal and paternal kin.

The question of why chimpanzees rely on olfaction when they have a high ability to recognize individuals and kin visually through facial cues is important for our understanding of the evolution of primate communication. Olfactory cues might be especially important when visibility is reduced. Most chimpanzees live in a dense forest habitat where the location and identification of conspecifics that are not close by may depend more on acoustic and odour cues rather than visual cues. Furthermore, chimpanzees sometimes feed, travel and mate at night [46,84–87], and olfaction may help to gather valuable social information in the dark. Finally, since olfactory cues can persist for longer than acoustic and visual cues [88], the ability to recognize individuals via olfaction may be especially important when the sender is no longer present.

Our results contribute to an active area of research on olfactory kin recognition mechanisms. Odour is linked to variation in major histocompatibility complex alleles and plays an important role in kin recognition in many mammalian species, including primates [89,79] (reviewed in [90]). Humans, for example, can discriminate between kin and non-kin via olfactory cues alone, and human mothers can recognize the odour of their own offspring [34,35]. Lemurs signal relatedness in their odour profiles [15,91] and detect this information via olfactory cues [16]. We provide the first behavioural evidence of odour-based kin recognition in apes, which is important to prevent infanticide and facilitate inbreeding avoidance and nepotism.

5. Conclusion

Olfaction in non-human apes has long been neglected. However, humans have retained good olfactory capabilities in the absence of a functional VNO and accessory olfactory bulb. Understanding how these human capabilities have evolved requires the study of the functional significance of odour cues in closely related species, which also lack these anatomical features. Our results provide behavioural evidence that olfaction plays a more important role in chimpanzee social life than hitherto suspected and suggest that chimpanzees obtain information about both inter- and intragroup social relationships from olfactory cues, filling an important gap in our understanding of primate chemical communication and contributing to the argument that a functional VNO is not necessary to perceive olfactory social signals. The ability to obtain information about conspecifics via odour may regulate chimpanzee behaviour and may be an adaptive advantage, for example via conflict management, mate choice, inbreeding avoidance, nepotism or the detection of ovulation. Future studies should investigate the full range of social information contained in ape odour sources using chemical analyses and further bioassays.

Ethics

The study was ethically approved by an internal committee at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Research and animal husbandry comply with the 'EAZA Minimum Standards for the Accommodation and Care of Animals in Zoos and Aquaria', the 'WAZA Ethical Guidelines for the Conduct of Research on Animals by Zoos and Aquariums' and the ASAB/ABS 'Guidelines for the Treatment of Animals in Behavioural Research and Teaching'.

Data accessibility

Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.0b3k209 [92].

Authors' contributions

S.H. conceived of, designed and coordinated the study, collected odour samples, conducted the bioassays, carried out the video and statistical analyses, and wrote and revised the manuscript. J.M.S. participated in the conception and design of the study and interpretation of the data and revised the manuscript. Both the authors gave their final approval for publication.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

S.H. was partially supported by a short-term PhD stipend from the State of Saxony (grant no. LAU-R-N-17-2-1016).

Acknowledgements

We thank Michael Tomasello for hosting S.H. at the Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology and Josep Call for his continuous support. We are grateful to all keepers at Leipzig Zoo for assistance in the behavioural experiments and the chimpanzees for their participation. We thank the staff at Wolfgang Köhler Primate Research Center, especially Daniel Hanus, Hanna Petschauer, Johannes Grossmann, Sebastian Schütte and Raik Pieszek for their support in the study. We are very grateful to Roger Mundry and Colleen Stephens for their invaluable statistical support. Many thanks also to Petra Jahn for IT support and Ramona Frickel for reliability coding. S.H. is grateful to Klaus Schildberger and Paul Stevenson for continuous support during her PhD studies and to the State of Saxony for a short-term PhD stipend. We are grateful for valuable feedback received at the Behaviour conference in Estoril, Portugal, 2017, and for helpful comments from two anonymous reviewers on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Footnotes

Electronic supplementary material is available online at https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.c.4258187.

© 2018 The Author(s)

Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.

References

  • 1.
    Pickenhagen W

    . 2017 History of odor and odorants. In Springer handbook of odor (ed.

    Buettner A

    ), pp. 5-6. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. (doi:10.1007/978-3-319-26932-0_1) Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 2.
    Stoddart DM

    . 1980 The ecology of vertebrate olfaction . Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer. Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 3.
    Wyatt TD

    . 2014 Pheromones and animal behavior: chemical signals and signatures , 2nd edn. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar

  • 4.
    Heymann EW

    . 2006 The neglected sense-olfaction in primate behavior, ecology, and evolution. Am. J. Primatol. 68 , 519-524. (doi:10.1002/ajp.20249) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 5.
    Drea CM

    . 2015 D'scent of man: a comparative survey of primate chemosignaling in relation to sex. Horm. Behav. 68 , 117-133. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.08.001) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 6.
    Nei M, Niimura Y, Nozawa M

    . 2008 The evolution of animal chemosensory receptor gene repertoires: roles of chance and necessity. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9 , 951-963. (doi:10.1038/Nrg2480) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 7.
    Baron G, Frahm HD, Bhatnagar KP, Stephan H

    . 1983 Comparison of brain structure volumes in insectivora and primates. III. Main olfactory bulb (MOB). J. Hirnforsch 24 , 551-568. PubMed, Google Scholar

  • 8.
    Gilad Y, Wiebe V, Przeworski M, Lancet D, Pääbo S

    . 2004 Loss of olfactory receptor genes coincides with the acquisition of full trichromatic vision in primates. PLoS Biol. 2 , e5. (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020005) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 9.
    Matsui A, Go Y, Niimura Y

    . 2010 Degeneration of olfactory receptor gene repertories in primates: no direct link to full trichromatic vision. Mol. Biol. Evol. 27 , 1192-1200. (doi:10.1093/molbev/msq003) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 10.
    Geissmann T

    . 1987 A sternal gland in the siamang gibbon (Hylobates syndactylus). Int. J. Primatol. 8 , 1-15. (doi:10.1007/BF02737111) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 11.
    delBarco-Trillo J, Drea CM

    . 2014 Socioecological and phylogenetic patterns in the chemical signals of strepsirrhine primates. Anim. Behav. 97 , 249-253. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.07.009) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 12.
    Ziegler TE, Epple G, Snowdon CT, Porter TA, Belcher AM, Küderling I

    . 1993 Detection of chemical signals of ovulation in the cotton-top tamarin, Saguinus oedipus . Anim. Behav. 45 , 313-322. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1993.1036) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 13.
    Smith TE, Abbott DH, Tomlinson AJ, Mlotkiewicz JA

    . 1997 Differential display of investigative behavior permits discrimination of scent signatures from familiar and unfamiliar socially dominant female marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). J. Chem. Ecol. 23 , 2523-2546. (doi:10.1023/B:JOEC.0000006664.38169.e5) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 14.
    Palagi E, Dapporto L

    . 2006 Beyond odor discrimination: demonstrating individual recognition by scent in Lemur catta . Chem. Senses 31 , 437-443. (doi:10.1093/chemse/bjj048) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 15.
    Charpentier MJE, Boulet M, Drea CM

    . 2008 Smelling right: the scent of male lemurs advertises genetic quality and relatedness. Mol. Ecol. 17 , 3225-3233. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.03831.x) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 16.
    Charpentier MJE, Crawford JC, Boulet M, Drea CM

    . 2010 Message 'scent': lemurs detect the genetic relatedness and quality of conspecifics via olfactory cues. Anim. Behav. 80 , 101-108. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.005) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 17.
    Smith TD, Laitman JT, Bhatnagar KP

    . 2014 The shrinking anthropoid nose, the human vomeronasal organ, and the language of anatomical reduction. Ana. Rec. 297 , 2196-2204. (doi:10.1002/ar.23035) Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 18.
    Keller M, Baum MJ, Brock O, Brennan PA, Bakker J

    . 2009 The main and the accessory olfactory systems interact in the control of mate recognition and sexual behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 200 , 268-276. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.01.020) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 19.
    Henkel S, Lambides AR, Berger A, Thomsen R, Widdig A

    . 2015 Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) recognize group membership via olfactory cues alone. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69 , 2019-2034. (doi:10.1007/s00265-015-2013-y) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 20.
    Vaglio S, Minicozzi P, Romoli R, Boscaro F, Pieraccini G, Moneti G, Moggi-Cecchi J

    . 2016 Sternal gland scent-marking signals sex, age, rank, and group identity in captive mandrills. Chem. Senses 41 , 177-186. (doi:10.1093/chemse/bjv077) PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 21.
    Weiß BM, Kücklich M, Thomsen R, Henkel S, Jänig S, Kulik L, Birkemeyer C, Widdig A

    . 2018 Chemical composition of axillary odorants reflects social and individual attributes in rhesus macaques. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 72 , 65. (doi:10.1007/s00265-018-2479-5) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 22.
    Rigaill L, Suda-Hashimoto N, Ducroix L, Mouri K, Furuichi T, Garcia C

    . 2017 Testing for links between female urine odor and male sexual behaviors in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Int. J. Primatol. 38 , 823-837. (doi:10.1007/s10764-017-9980-y) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 23.
    Clarke PMR, Barrett L, Henzi SP

    . 2009 What role do olfactory cues play in chacma baboon mating? Am. J. Primatol. 71 , 493-502. (doi:10.1002/ajp.20678) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 24.
    Rigaill L, Higham JP, Lee PC, Blin A, Garcia C

    . 2013 Multimodal sexual signaling and mating behavior in olive baboons (Papio anubis). Am. J. Primatol. 75 , 774-787. (doi:10.1002/ajp.22154) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 25.
    Setchell JM, Vaglio S, Moggi-Cecchi J, Boscaro F, Calamai L, Knapp LA

    . 2010 Chemical composition of scent-gland secretions in an Old World monkey (Mandrillus sphinx): influence of sex, male status, and individual identity. Chem. Senses 35 , 205-220. (doi:10.1093/chemse/bjp105) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 26.
    Poirotte C, Massol F, Herbert A, Willaume E, Bomo PM, Kappeler PM, Charpentier MJE

    . 2017 Mandrills use olfaction to socially avoid parasitized conspecifics. Sci. Adv. 3 , e1601721. (doi:10.1126/sciadv.1601721) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 27.
    Shepherd GM

    . 2004 The human sense of smell: are we better than we think? PLoS Biol. 2 , e146. (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020146) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 28.
    McGann JP

    . 2017 Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science 356 , eaam7263. (doi:10.1126/science.aam7263) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 29.
    Bushdid C, Magnasco MO, Vosshall LB, Keller A

    . 2014 Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Science 343 , 1370-1372. (doi:10.1126/science.1249168) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 30.
    Laska M

    . 2017 Human and animal olfactory capabilities compared. In Springer handbook of odor (ed.

    Buettner A

    ), pp. 81-82. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 31.
    Havlicek J, Roberts SC

    . 2009 MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34 , 497-512. (doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 32.
    Jaworska J, Rapacz-Leonard A, Janowski TE, Jezierski T

    . 2017 Does the major histocompatibility complex influence choice of mate in humans and other mammals? A review. Animal Sci. Papers Reports 35 , 107-122. ISI, Google Scholar

  • 33.
    Porter RH

    . 1991 Mutual mother-infant recognition in humans. In Kin recognition (ed

    Hepper PG

    ), pp. 413-432. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 34.
    Porter RH

    . 1999 Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica 1004 , 259-263. Google Scholar

  • 35.
    Weisfeld GE, Czilli T, Phillips KA, Gall JA, Lichtman CM

    . 2003 Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 85 , 279-295. (doi:10.1016/S0022-0965(03)00061-4) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 36.
    Jänig S, Weiß BM, Widdig A

    . 2018 Comparing the sniffing behavior of great apes. Am. J. Primatol. 80 , e22872. (doi:10.1002/ajp.22872) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 37.
    Hepper PG, Wells DL

    . 2012 Olfactory discrimination in the western lowland gorilla, Gorilla gorilla gorilla . Primates 53 , 121-126. (doi:10.1007/s10329-011-0291-1) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 38.
    Sarabian C, Belais R, MacIntosh AJJ

    . 2018 Feeding decisions under contamination risk in bonobos. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373 , 20170195. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0195) Link, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 39.
    Sarabian C, Ngoubangoye B, MacIntosh AJJ

    . 2017 Sarabian et al. 2017 avoidance of biological contaminants through sight, smell and touch in chimpanzees. R. Soc. open Sci. 4 , 170968. (doi:10.1098/rsos.170968) Link, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 40.
    Hepper PG, Wells DL

    . 2010 Individually identifiable body odors are produced by the gorilla and discriminated by humans. Chem. Senses 35 , 263-268. (doi:10.1093/chemse/bjq015) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 41.
    Klailova M, Lee PC

    . 2014 Wild western lowland gorillas signal selectively using odor. PLoS ONE 9 , e99554. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099554) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 42.
    Roffman I, Nevo E

    . 2010 Can chimpanzee biology highlight human origin and evolution? Rambam Maimonides Med. J. 1 , e0009. (doi:10.5041/RMMJ.10009) Crossref, PubMed, Google Scholar

  • 43.
    Matsumoto-Oda A, Kutsukake N, Hosaka K, Matsusaka T

    . 2007 Sniffing behaviors in Mahale chimpanzees. Primates 48 , 81-85. (doi:10.1007/s10329-006-0006-1) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 44.
    Watts DP, Mitani JC

    . 2001 Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138 , 299-327. (doi:10.1163/15685390152032488) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 45.
    Mitani JC, Watts DP

    . 2005 Correlates of territorial boundary patrol behaviour in wild chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 70 , 1079-1086. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.012) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 46.
    Goodall J

    . 1986 The chimpanzees of gombe: patterns of behavior . Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University. Google Scholar

  • 47.
    Boesch C

    . 1996 Social grouping in Taï chimpanzees. In Great ape societies (eds

    McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T

    ), pp. 101-113. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 48.
    Boesch C, Crockford C, Herbinger I, Wittig R, Moebius Y, Normand E

    . 2008 Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Taï National Park: lethal violence and the female perspective. Am. J. Primatol. 70 , 519-532. (doi:10.1002/ajp.20524) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 49.
    Pusey AE, Wolf M

    . 1996 Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 11 , 201-206. (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)10028-8) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 50.
    Widdig A

    . 2007 Paternal kin discrimination: the evidence and likely mechanisms. Biol. Rev. 82 , 319-334. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00011.x) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 51.
    Lehmann J, Fickenscher G, Boesch C

    . 2006 Kin biased investment in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 143 , 931-955. (doi:10.1163/156853906778623635) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 52.
    Gilby I, Wrangham R

    . 2008 Association patterns among wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) reflect sex differences in cooperation. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62 , 1831-1842. (doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0612-6) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 53.
    Mitani JC

    . 2009 Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Anim. Behav. 77 , 633-640. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.11.021) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 54.
    Murray CM, Stanton MA, Lonsdorf EV, Wroblewski EE, Pusey AE

    . 2016 Chimpanzee fathers bias their behaviour towards their offspring. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3 , 160441. (doi:10.1098/rsos.160441) Link, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 55.
    Langergraber K, Mitani J, Vigilant L

    . 2007 The limited impact of kinship on cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104 , 7786-7790. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0611449104) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 56.
    Walker KK, Rudicell RS, Li Y, Hahn BH, Wroblewski E, Pusey AE

    . 2017 Chimpanzees breed with genetically dissimilar mates. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4 , 160422. (doi:10.1098/rsos.160422) Link, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 57.
    Parr LA, de Waal FBM

    . 1999 Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees. Nature 399 , 647-648. (doi:10.1038/21345) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 58.
    Parr LA, Winslow JT, Hopkins WD, de Waal FBM

    . 2000 Recognizing facial cues: individual discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J. Comp. Psychol. 114 , 1-14. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.47) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 59.
    Martin P, Bateson P

    . 2007 Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide, updated edition . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 60.
    Baayen H

    . 2008 Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R , 1st edn. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 61.

    R Core Team. 2017 R: a language and environment for statistical computing . Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Google Scholar

  • 62.
    Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S

    . 2015 Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67 , 1-48. (doi:10.18637/jss.v067.i01) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 63.
    Aujard F, Némoz-Bertholet F

    . 2004 Response to urinary volatiles and chemosensory function decline with age in a prosimian primate. Physiol. Behav. 81 , 639-644. (doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.03.003) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 64.
    Schiffmann SS

    . 1997 Taste and smell losses in normal aging and disease. JAMA 278 , 1357-1362. (doi:10.1001/jama.1997.03550160077042) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 65.
    Drea CM, Boulet M, Delbarco-Trillo J, Greene LK, Sacha CR, Goodwin TE, Dubay GR

    . 2013 The 'secret' in secretions: methodological considerations in deciphering primate olfactory communication. Am. J. Primatol. 75 , 621-642. (doi:10.1002/ajp.22143) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 66.
    Demaria C, Roeder J-J

    . 1989 Responses to Urinary Stimuli in Pigtailed (Macaca nemestrina) and Stumptailed (Macaca arctoides) Macaques. Primates 30 , 111-115. (doi:10.1007/BF02381217) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 67.
    Dunbar I, Carmichael M

    . 1981 The response of male dogs to urine from other males. Behav. Neural Biol. 31 , 465-470. (doi:10.1016/S0163-1047(81)91546-6) Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 68.
    Drea CM, Vignieri SN, Kim HS, Weldele ML, Glickmann SE

    . 2002 Responses to olfactory stimuli in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta): II. Discrimination of conspecific scent. J. Comp. Psychol. 116 , 342-349. (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.116.4.342) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 69.
    Péron F, Ward R, Burman O

    . 2014 Horses (Equus caballus) discriminate body odour cues from conspecifics. Anim. Cogn. 17 , 1007-1011. (doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0717-9) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 70.
    Cinková I, Policht R

    . 2015 Discrimination of familiarity and sex from chemical cues in the dung by wild southern white rhinoceros. Anim. Cogn. 18 , 385-392. (doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0810-8) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 71.
    Fornasieri I, Roeder J-J

    . 1992 Behavioral responses to own and other species' scent marks in Lemur fulvus and Lemur macaco. J. Chem. Ecol. 18 , 2069-2082. (doi:10.1007/BF00981928) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 72.
    Ramsay NF, Giller PS

    . 1996 Scent-marking in ring-tailed lemurs: responses to the introduction of 'foreign' scent in the home range. Primates 37 , 13-23. (doi:10.1007/BF02382916) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 73.
    Ueno Y

    . 1994 Olfactory discrimination of urine odors from five species by tufted capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates 35 , 311-323. (doi:10.1007/BF02382728) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 74.
    Gosling LM

    . 1982 A reassessment of the function of scent marking in territories. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 60 , 89-118. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1982.tb00492.x) Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 75.
    Gosling LM, Roberts SC

    . 2001 Scent-marking by male mammals: cheat-proof signals to competitors and mates. Adv. Study Behav. 30 , 169-217. (doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(01)80007-3) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 76.
    Safi K, Kerth G

    . 2003 Secretions of the interaural gland contain information about individuality and colony membership in the Bechstein's bat. Anim. Behav. 65 , 363-369. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2067) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 77.
    Stoffel MA, Caspers BA, Forcada J, Giannakara A, Baier M, Eberhart-Phillips L, Müller C, Hoffman JI

    . 2015 Chemical fingerprints encode mother–offspring similarity, colony membership, relatedness, and genetic quality in fur seals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112 , E5005-E5012. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1506076112) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 78.
    Leclaire S, Jacob S, Greene LK, Dubay GR, Drea CM

    . 2017 Social odours covary with bacterial community in the anal secretions of wild meerkats. Sci. Rep. 7 , 1-13. (doi:10.1038/s41598-017-03356-x) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 79.
    Setchell JM, Vaglio S, Abbott KM, Moggi-Cecchi J, Boscaro F, Pieraccini G, Knapp LA

    . 2011 Odour signals major histocompatibility complex genotype in an Old World monkey. Proc. R. Soc. B 278 , 274-280. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.0571) Link, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 80.
    Langergraber KE, Mitani JC, Vigilant L

    . 2009 Kinship and social bonds in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am. J. Primatol. 71 , 1-12. (doi:10.1002/ajp.20711) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 81.
    Pusey AE

    . 1980 Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees. Anim. Behav. 28 , 543-552. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80063-7) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 82.
    Silk JB

    . 2009 Nepotistic cooperation in non-human primate groups. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364 , 3243. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0118) Link, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 83.
    Parr LA, Heintz M, Lonsdorf E, Wroblewski E

    . 2010 Visual kin recognition in nonhuman primates: (Pan troglodytes and Macaca mulatta): inbreeding avoidance or male distinctiveness? J. Comp. Psychol. 124 , 343-350. (doi:10.1037/a0020545) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 84.
    Goodall J

    . 1968 The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 1 , 165-311. Google Scholar

  • 85.
    Pruetz J, Bertolani P

    . 2009 Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) behavioral responses to stresses associated with living in a Savannah-Mosaic environment: implications for hominin adaptations to open habitats. Paleoanthropology 2009 , 252-262. (doi:10.4207/PA.2009.ART33) Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 86.
    Zamma K

    . 2014 What makes wild chimpanzees wake up at night? Primates 55 , 51-57. (doi:10.1007/s10329-013-0367-1) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 87.
    Nishida T

    . 1996 Review of recent findings on Mahale chimpanzees. In Chimpanzee cultures (ed.

    Wrangham RW

    ), pp. 373-396. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univeristy Press. Google Scholar

  • 88.
    Eisenberg JF, Kleiman DG

    . 1972 Olfactory communication in mammals. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 3 , 1. (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.03.110172.000245) Crossref, Google Scholar

  • 89.
    Boulet M, Charpentier M, Drea C

    . 2009 Decoding an olfactory mechanism of kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance in a primate. BMC Evol. Biol. 9 , 281. (doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-281) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 90.
    Brown JL, Eklund A

    . 1994 Kin recognition and the major histocompatibility complex: an integrative review. Am. Nat. 143 , 435-461. (doi:10.1086/285612) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 91.
    Morelli TL, Hayes RA, Nahrung HF, Goodwin TE, Harelimana IH, MacDonald LJ, Wright PC

    . 2013 Relatedness communicated in lemur scent. Naturwissenschaften 100 , 769-777. (doi:10.1007/s00114-013-1074-x) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

  • 92.
    Henkel S, Setchell JM

    . 2018

    Data from: Group and kin recognition via olfactory cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

    . Dryad Digital Repository . (doi:10.5061/dryad.0b3k209) Google Scholar

How Do Great Apes Keep Their Babies Clean From Urine and Feces

Source: https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/10.1098/rspb.2018.1527

0 Response to "How Do Great Apes Keep Their Babies Clean From Urine and Feces"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel